Managing Cognitive Bias

December 18, 2024
  • Capital Partners
BBH Senior Wealth Planner Ross Bruch looks at investment-related biases and offers solutions to help recognize and minimize their negative effects.

The endowment effect

In 1990, behavioral economists Daniel Kahneman, Jack Knetsch, and Richard Thaler published the results of a now-famous experiment commonly known among behavioral scientists as “the mug study.”1 The researchers divided Cornell University student volunteers into two equal groups. The first group received a coffee mug from the university bookstore; the other group received nothing. The experimenters then created a market between the groups to allow students with mugs to sell them to the students without mugs.

Traditional economic theory predicts that under such circumstances, the parties should be able to find a price at which about half the mugs will exchange hands. However, in the experiment, very few mugs were sold due to a large gap in valuations; on average, students that were given the mug demanded more than twice the price that the students without the mug were willing to pay.

Why did two similar groups of people differ so widely in their pricing? Based on the results of this study, which has been repeated and verified through additional experiments numerous times over the past three decades, the researchers determined that people place a higher value on things they own compared with things they don’t, whether it be a mug, a car, or an investment. The researchers labeled this phenomenon the “endowment effect.”

The endowment effect is a type of cognitive bias, or an error in thinking that occurs when people incorrectly process and interpret information in the world around them. In other words, it’s what happens when one’s brain creates a new “subjective reality,” ignoring or minimizing its focus on objective input and, at times, leading people to make illogical choices.

This phenomenon is particularly relevant to investment decisions because it has the capability to interfere with one’s ability to correctly evaluate market risk. It can also lead individuals to be overly attached to their investments. For example, a person who inherits shares of stock from a deceased relative may exhibit the endowment effect by refusing to sell those shares, even if they do not fit within the beneficiary’s investment goals or portfolio diversification.

The endowment effect is one of many types of cognitive biases that can irrationally influence investment decisions. Here, we highlight additional types of investment-related biases and offer three possible solutions to help recognize and minimize their negative effects.

Prospect theory

Imagine a gameshow contestant who has just won a $10,000 prize; he is then offered a chance to double his winnings by correctly calling the outcome of a coinflip, but he also risks losing it all if he guesses incorrectly. Should he opt for this double-or-nothing opportunity?

From a strictly economic viewpoint, these two options are equal. On average, people that flip the coin will receive $10,000, since half the group will win $20,000, and the other half will walk away empty-handed. Yet when faced with this type of decision, most people take the guaranteed payment. In fact, people will often “play it safe” even if the economic odds slightly favor taking a risk – for example, if the top prize in the gameshow situation is raised to $21,000, most people will still choose a $10,000 prize.

The leading theory behind why people typically prefer the safer option – known as prospect theory – states that the mental pain of a loss outweighs the joy of an equally valued gain.2 In the case of the coinflip, guessing incorrectly feels like losing $10,000, even though contestants walk away no worse off than they were before entering the contest. And while doubling one’s payout following a correct guess feels good, the economic benefit one receives in this situation doesn’t match the possible psychological detriment of receiving nothing.

Other cognitive biases

While the endowment effect and prospect theory are among the most well-known cognitive biases, they are far from the only ones that can affect financial decisions and risk assessments. Other notable cognitive biases include the following:

  • Overconfidence bias occurs when one overestimates one’s abilities and knowledge, thereby causing him or her to misjudge the likelihood of a successful outcome.
  • Confirmation bias happens when one seeks out information that supports one’s existing beliefs and ignores information that contradicts those beliefs, thereby causing a “blind spot” in his or her analysis.
  • Sunk-cost bias is the tendency to continue investing time, money, or other resources into a situation because of the resources that have already been invested, even if the situation is not likely to be successful.
  • Availability bias occurs when one bases judgments on information that is easily available, rather than on all of the relevant information.
  • Anchoring bias is the tendency to rely too heavily on the first piece of information that is provided and to use that as a reference point for all subsequent judgments.

Managing cognitive biases

Cognitive biases aren’t always counterproductive. In many instances, they provide rules of thumb or information processing shortcuts so people can evaluate new data more quickly and easily to make both major and minor decisions. However, when we misunderstand our instincts or they cause us to misinterpret information, they become harmful and can lead to poor risk analysis and decision-making.

To a large degree, such biases are impossible, and frequently undesirable, to eradicate entirely. Yet it is important to find ways to manage these biases and mitigate their potential damage to your long-term planning and investing goals. Here are three evidence-based methods to help limit the negative impact of cognitive biases:

Recognizing the existence of cognitive biases and examining whether they may influence decisions can be a helpful step in reducing their potential negative effect. A literature review on critical thinking and cognitive bias found that metacognition – having an awareness and understanding of one’s own thought process – was helpful in reducing the impact of such biases.3 However, to do so, one must first understand the potential types of biases that can influence decisions. While this article has identified several of the most prevalent forms of biases in financial decisions, it is far from an exhaustive list. Additional exploration in the psychology of bias is highly encouraged.

Research suggests that financial literacy and education play an important role in reducing cognitive bias and thus empowering and enabling individuals to make smarter decisions about money and wealth.4 While this approach is especially applicable to new or inexperienced investors, experienced investors may also benefit from recognizing its importance when considering investments in financial sectors they are less familiar with.

It is very difficult for decision-makers to separate themselves from a specific situation when they are emotionally tied to the outcome. Instead, research has demonstrated that an effective tool in limiting cognitive biases is to have decision-makers “consider the opposite” or “consider an alternative.”5 Using this strategy, decision-makers are asked to generate reasons for an opposing position or explain an alternative outcome – essentially trying to argue with their own beliefs. Doing so directs their attention to reasons for the alternative positions rather than simply generating supporting reasons for the initially held position.

All three of these mitigation tools force decision-makers to slow down, think through their decision process, and consider a more well-rounded, well-informed approach. While these solutions may be inefficient for making insignificant or minor choices, employing them to minimize cognitive bias can be helpful in improving decisions that have long-term consequences.

Contact Us

Up Next
Up Next

Pillars of Persistence: Strategies for Building Work Ethic and Determination

Senior Wealth Planner Ross Bruch looks at the science behind how business owners can pass down motivation and determination to younger generations.

1 Kahneman, Daniel, Jack L. Knetsch, and Richard H. Thaler. “Experimental Tests of the Endowment Effect and the Coase Theorem.” Journal of Political Economy.
2 Tversky, Amos, and Daniel Kahneman. “Advances in Prospect Theory: Cumulative Representation of Uncertainty.” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 5, no. 4 (1992): 297–323.
3 Maynes, Jeffrey. “Critical Thinking and Cognitive Bias.” Informal Logic 35, no. 2 (2015): 183–203.
4 Khan, Dahiman. “Cognitive Driven Biases, Investment Decision Making: The Moderating Role of Financial Literacy.” January 5, 2020.
5 Hirt, Edward R., and Keith D. Markman. “Multiple Explanation: A Consider-an-Alternative Strategy for Debiasing Judgments.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69, no. 6 (1995): 1069–86.

Brown Brothers Harriman & Co. (“BBH”) may be used to reference the company as a whole and/or its various subsidiaries generally.  This material and any products or services may be issued or provided in multiple jurisdictions by duly authorized and regulated subsidiaries.  This material is for general information and reference purposes only and does not constitute legal, tax or investment advice and is not intended as an offer to sell, or a solicitation to buy securities, services or investment products.  Any reference to tax matters is not intended to be used, and may not be used, for purposes of avoiding penalties under the U.S. Internal Revenue Code, or other applicable tax regimes, or for promotion, marketing or recommendation to third parties. All information has been obtained from sources believed to be reliable, but accuracy is not guaranteed, and reliance should not be placed on the information presented.  This material may not be reproduced, copied or transmitted, or any of the content disclosed to third parties, without the permission of BBH.  All trademarks and service marks included are the property of BBH or their respective owners. © Brown Brothers Harriman & Co. 2024.  All rights reserved. PB-07746-2024-09-13

As of June 15, 2022 Internet Explorer 11 is not supported by BBH.com.

Important Information for Non-U.S. Residents

You are required to read the following important information, which, in conjunction with the Terms and Conditions, governs your use of this website. Your use of this website and its contents constitute your acceptance of this information and those Terms and Conditions. If you do not agree with this information and the Terms and Conditions, you should immediately cease use of this website. The contents of this website have not been prepared for the benefit of investors outside of the United States. This website is not intended as a solicitation of the purchase or sale of any security or other financial instrument or any investment management services for any investor who resides in a jurisdiction other than the United States1. As a general matter, Brown Brothers Harriman & Co. and its subsidiaries (“BBH”) is not licensed or registered to solicit prospective investors and offer investment advisory services in jurisdictions outside of the United States. The information on this website is not intended to be distributed to, directed at or used by any person or entity in any jurisdiction or country where such distribution or use would be contrary to law or regulation. Persons in respect of whom such prohibitions apply must not access the website.  Under certain circumstances, BBH may provide services to investors located outside of the United States in accordance with applicable law. The conditions under which such services may be provided will be analyzed on a case-by-case basis by BBH. BBH will only accept investors from such jurisdictions or countries where it has made a determination that such an arrangement or relationship is permissible under the laws of that jurisdiction or country. The existence of this website is not intended to be a substitute for the type of analysis described above and is not intended as a solicitation of or recommendation to any prospective investor, including those located outside of the United States. Certain BBH products or services may not be available in certain jurisdictions. By choosing to access this website from any location other than the United States, you accept full responsibility for compliance with all local laws. The website contains content that has been obtained from sources that BBH believes to be reliable as of the date presented; however, BBH cannot guarantee the accuracy of such content, assure its completeness, or warrant that such information will not be changed. The content contained herein is current as of the date of issuance and is subject to change without notice. The website’s content does not constitute investment advice and should not be used as the basis for any investment decision. There is no guarantee that any investment objectives, expectations, targets described in this website or the  performance or profitability of any investment will be achieved. You understand that investing in securities and other financial instruments involves risks that may affect the value of the securities and may result in losses, including the potential loss of the principal invested, and you assume and are able to bear all such risks.  In no event shall BBH or any other affiliated party be liable for any direct, incidental, special, consequential, indirect, lost profits, loss of business or data, or punitive damages arising out of your use of this website. By clicking accept, you confirm that you accept  to the above Important Information along with Terms and Conditions.

 
1BBH sponsors UCITS Funds registered in Luxembourg, in certain jurisdictions. For information on those funds, please see bbhluxembourgfunds.com



captcha image

Type in the word seen on the picture

I am a current investor in another jurisdiction